Dimensions

PlumX

How to Cite
Pérez-Acosta, A. M., Martínez, G. S., Corredor Páez, K. E., Vargas-Irwin, C., & Méndez Pardo, L. F. (2016). Consummatory Successive Negative Contrast in Mongolian Gerbil (Meriones unguiculatus). International Journal of Psychological Research, 9(2), 52–60. https://doi.org/10.21500/20112084.2425
License terms
The work that is sent to this journal must be original, not published or sent to be published elsewhere; and if it is accepted for publication, authors will agree to transfer copyright to International Journal of Psychological Research. 

To give up copyright, the authors allow that, International Journal of Psychological Research, distribute the work more broadly, check for the reuse by others and take care of the necessary procedures for the registration and administration of copyright; at the same time, our editorial board represents the interests of the author and allows authors to re-use his work in various forms. In response to the above, authors transfer copyright to the journal, International Journal of Psychological Research. This transfer does not imply other rights which are not those of authorship (for example those that concern about patents). Likewise, preserves the authors rights to use the work integral or partially in lectures, books and courses, as well as make copies for educational purposes. Finally, the authors may use freely the tables and figures in its future work, wherever make explicit reference to the previous publication in International Journal of Psychological Research. The assignment of copyright includes both virtual rights and forms of the article to allow the editorial to disseminate the work in the manner which it deems appropriate. 

The editorial board reserves the right of amendments deemed necessary in the application of the rules of publication.

Abstract

Successive Negative Contrast (SNC) is a phenomenon in which there is a sharp decrease in the rate of reinforced behavior immediately after a sudden reduction in quality or quantity of a habitual reinforcer. This phenomenon has become a solid experimental paradigm for the study of frustration in mammals but it has not been reported to date in the Mongolian gerbil (Meriones unguiculatus). This species is monogamous and biparental, and these differential characteristics make it interesting for comparative analysis. This work includes modifications to the SNC paradigm for this species, assessing its response to sudden changes in positive reinforcement and its differences by sex. In the first phase of the experiment, a multiple choice test was used to identify the preferred reinforcer for the species (sesame paste). After the selection of the primary incentive, the contrast procedure was carried out by presenting sesame paste in the pre-shift phase and LabDiet Laboratory Rodent Diet food during post-shift one. A repeated measures ANOVA of showed a significant reduction of consummatory behavior in the experimental group when compared with the control group. No differences were observed by sex. These data expand the possibility of using gerbils in the comparative study of the relationship between learning and emotion in mammals, both in behavioral and in neurobiological levels.

References

Amsel, A. (2006). Frustration Theory: An Analysis of Dispositional Learning and Memory. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press.

Bentosela, M., Jakovcevic, A., Elgier, A. M., Mustaca, A. E., & Papini, M. R. (2009). Incentive contrast in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 123(2), 125-130. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0013340

Crespi, L. P. (1942). Quantitative Variation of Incentive and Performance in the White Rat. The American Journal of Psychology, 55(4), 467-517. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1417120

Cuenya, L., Kamenetzky, G. V., Fosacheca, S., & Mustaca, A. E. (2013). Successive negative contrast effect on the emotional evaluation of images. Anales de Psicología, 29(3), 944-952. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.6018/analesps.29.3.150861

Dantzer, R., Arnone, M., & Mormede, P. (1980). Effects of frustration on behaviour and plasma corticosteroid levels in pigs. Physiology & Behavior, 24(1), 1-4. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(80)90005-0

Elliott, M. H. (1928). The Effect of Change of Reward on the Maze Performance of Rats (Vol. 4). Berkeley: University of California Press.

Flaherty, C. F. (1996). Incentive relativity. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Flórez-Acevedo, S., Sarmiento, N., & Martínez, G. S. (2010). Patrones de alimentación, sueño y actividad reproductiva en Jerbos de Mongolia. Suma Psicológica, 17(2), 201-208.

Freidin, E., Cuello, M. I., & Kacelnik, A. (2009). Successive negative contrast in a bird: starlings' behaviour after unpredictable negative changes in food quality. Animal Behaviour, 77(4), 857-865. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.12.010

Gulotta, E. F. (1971). Meriones unguiculatus. Mammalian Species(3), 1-5. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/3503988

Herrnstein, R. J. (1970). On the law of effect. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 13(2), 243-266. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1901%2Fjeab.1970.13-243

Hwang, I. K., Lee, Y.-B., Yoo, K.-Y., Kang, T.-C., Kim, D.-W., Moon, W.-K., . . . Won, M. H. (2005). Seizure-induced changes of mineralocorticoid and glucocorticoid receptors in the hippocampus in seizure sensitive gerbils. Neuroscience Research, 53(1), 14-24. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neures.2005.05.006

Justel, N. R., Ruetti, E. M., & Mustaca, A. (2010). Aproximaciones teóricas al estudio del contraste sucesivo negativo consumatorio. Suma Psicológica, 17(2), 111-123.

Manzo, L., Donaire, R., Sabariego, M., Papini, M. R., & Torres, C. (2015). Anti-anxiety self-medication in rats: oral consumption of chlordiazepoxide and ethanol after reward devaluation. Behavioural Brain Research, 278, 90-97. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2014.09.017

Martínez, G. S. (2008). El maltrato infantil: mecanismos subyacentes. Avances en Psicología Clínica Latinoamericana, 26(2), 171-179.

Martínez, G. S., Vargas-Irwin, C., & Pérez Acosta, A. M. (2011). Evalución del efecto conductual de la omisión sorpresiva de reforzamiento en jerbos. International Journal of Psychological Research, 4(Número especial: Memorias del XXXIII Congreso Interamericano de Psicología, Medellín), 1621.

Mast, V. K., Fagen, J. W., Rovee-Collier, C. K., & Sullivan, M. W. (1980). Immediate and long-term memory for reinforcement context: the development of learned expectancies in early infancy. Child Development, 51(3), 700-707. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1129455

Muller, J. M., Moore, H., Myers, M. M., & Shair, H. N. (2009). Dopamine's role in social modulation of infant isolation-induced vocalization: II. Maternally modulated infant separation responses are regulated by D1- and D2-family dopamine receptors. Developmental Psychobiology, 51(2), 158-172. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1002%2Fdev.20355

Mustaca, A., Bentosela, M., & Papini, M. R. (2000). Consummatory Successive Negative Contrast in Mice. Learning and Motivation, 31, 272-282. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/lmot.2000.1055

Norris, J. N., Pérez-Acosta, A. M., Ortega, L. A., & Papini, M. R. (2009). Naloxone facilitates appetitive extinction and eliminates escape from frustration. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, & Behavior, 94(1), 81-87. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pbb.2009.07.012

Olszewski, P. K., & Levine, A. S. (2007). Central opioids and sweet tastants: When reward outweighs homeostasis. Physiology and Behavior, 91(5), 506-512. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.01.011

Ortega, L. A., Prado-Rivera, M. A., Cárdenas-Poveda, D. C., McLinden, K. A., Glueck, A. C., Gutiérrez, G., . . . Papini, M. R. (2013). Tests of the aversive summation hypothesis in rats: Effects of restraint stress on consummatory successive negative contrast and extinction in the Barnes maze. Learning and Motivation, 44(3), 159-173. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lmot.2013.02.001

Papini, M. R. (2003). Comparative psychology of surprising nonreward. Brain, Behavior & Evolution, 62(2), 83-95. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000072439

Papini, M. R. (2008). Comparative psychology. Evolution and development of behavior (2nd ed.). New York: Psychology Press.

Papini, M. R., Wood, M., Daniel, A. M., & Norris, J. N. (2006). Reward loss as psychological pain. International Journal of Psychology & Psychological Therapy, 6(2), 189-213.

Razzoli, M., & Valsecchi, P. (2006). Different social bonds produce differential effects on behaviour and physiology in Mongolian gerbils. Ethology Ecology & Evolution, 18(4), 289-306. doi:doi/abs/10.1080/08927014.2006.9522697

Ruetti, E. M., & Justel, N. R. (2010). Bases neurobiológicas de la frustración. Revista Argentina de Ciencias del Comportamiento, 2(3), 45-60.

Thorndike, E. (1911). Animal intelligence: Experimental studies. New York: The Macmillan company.

Tinklepaugh, O. L. (1928). An experimental study of representative factors in monkeys. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 8(3), 197-236.

Vogel, J. R., Mikulka, P. J., & Spear, N. E. (1968). Effects of shifts in sucrose and saccharine concentrations on licking behavior in the rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 66(3), 661-666. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0026556

Woolley, M. L., Haman, M., Higgins, G. A., & Ballard, T. M. (2006). Investigating the effect of bilateral amygdala lesions on fear conditioning and social interaction in the male Mongolian gerbil. Brain Research, 1078(1), 151-158. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2006.01.071

Wynne-Edwards, K. E. (2007). Paternal care in rodents: Weakening support for hormonal regulation of the transition to behavioral fatherhood in rodent animal models of biparental care. Hormones & Behavior, 52(1), 114-121. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.03.018

Downloads

Download data is not yet available.

Cited by